It can be the smallest event that triggers Donna Penner’s traumatic memories of an operation from more than ten years ago.
One day, for instance, she was waiting in the car as her daughter ran an errand, and realized that she was trapped inside. What might once have been a frustrating inconvenience sent her into a panic attack. “I started screaming. I was flailing my arms, I was crying,” she says. “It just left me so shaken.”
Even the wrong clothing can make her anxiety worse. “Anything that’s tight around my neck is out of the question because it makes me feel like I’m suffocating,” says Donna, a 55-year-old from Altona in Manitoba, Canada.
Donna would not be like this if it were not for a small medical procedure that she had before her 45th birthday. She was working in the accountancy department of a local trucking company and had just celebrated the wedding of one of her daughters. But she had been having severe bleeding and pain during her period, and her family physician had suggested that they investigate the causes with exploratory surgery.
It should have been a routine procedure, but, for reasons that are far from clear, the general anesthetic failed. Rather than lying in peaceful oblivion, she woke up just before the surgeon made the first cut into her abdomen. With her body still paralyzed by the anesthetic drugs, she was unable to signal that anything was wrong.
So she remained frozen and helpless on the operating table as the surgeon probed her body, while she experienced indescribable agony. “I thought, ‘This is it, this is how I’m going to die, right here on the table, and my family will never know what my last few hours were like because no one’s even noticing what’s going on.’”
The lingering trauma can resurface with the slightest trigger, and still causes her to have “two or three nightmares each night”. Having been put on medical leave from her job, she has lost her independence. She suspects that she will never fully escape the effects of that day more than a decade ago. “It’s a life sentence.”
For years, anesthesia awareness has been shrouded in mystery. Although extreme experiences like Donna’s are rare, there is now evidence that around 5 per cent of people may wake up on the operating table – and possibly many more.
Thanks to the amnesiac effects of the drugs, however, most of these people will be unable to remember anything about the event – and whether or not that is something we should be concerned about is both a practical and a philosophical question.
These results are all the more significant given just how often general anesthesia is now used. “Almost three million general anesthetics happen each year in the UK alone,” says Peter Odor, a registrar at St George’s Hospital in London. “As a consequence, it is more probable than not that someone, somewhere in the world, right now is aware during their surgery.”
We once knew surprisingly little about why anesthesia works. Now, however, researchers are striving to understand more about the nature of going under and the circumstances in which anesthesia doesn’t work, in the hope of making advances that might reduce the risk of anesthesia awareness. And, with a greater understanding of the anesthetized state, we may even be able to turn a rudimentary awareness to our advantage – in the form of medical hypnosis.
Let’s be clear: anesthesia is a medical miracle. Since at least the time of Hippocrates, physicians and medicine men had hunted for a good way to ease the pain of medical procedures. While drugs such as alcohol, opium and even hemlock could act as sedatives, their efficacy was unreliable; most patients did not escape the torture.
By the 1840s, scientists had discovered various gases that appeared to have sedative effects. One of these, sulfuric ether, had attracted the particular attention of a dentist based in Boston, named William Morton, who put it to the test in a public demonstration at the Massachusetts General Hospital in 1846. Although the patient was still able to mutter half-coherent thoughts, he reportedly felt no pain, just the faint sense of his skin being “scratched with a hoe”.
The news of the demonstration soon spread throughout the medical establishment, heralding the start of the anesthetic era. With the subsequent discovery of even more effective anesthetic agents such as chloroform, the agony of the surgical knife seemed to be a thing of the past.
General anesthesia creates a controlled unconsciousness that is deeper and more detached from reality even than sleep
Today, anesthetists have a wide range of pain-killing and consciousness-reducing drugs at their disposal, and the exact choice will depend on the procedure and the patient’s particular needs.
Often, the aim is not to produce a loss of consciousness but simply to remove the sensation from a particular part of the body. So-called regional anesthetics include spinal and epidural anesthetics, both of which are delivered between the bones of your back to numb the lower half of your body. These are commonly used during childbirth, bladder operations and hip replacements.
You may also be given a sedative – which produces a relaxed, sleepy state but does not fully eliminate your awareness.
General anesthesia, in contrast, aims to do just that, creating an unresponsive drug-induced coma or controlled unconsciousness that is deeper and more detached from reality even than sleep, with no memories of any events during that period. As Robert Sanders, an anesthetist at the University of Wisconsin–Madison, puts it: “We’ve apparently ablated this period of time from that person’s experience.”
We still don’t know exactly why anesthetic agents dim our consciousness, but they are thought to interfere with various brain chemicals called neurotransmitters. These chemicals turn up or turn down the activity of neurons, particularly the widespread communication between different brain regions.
Propofol, for instance – a milky-white fluid used in general anesthetics and some types of sedation – seems to amplify the effects of GABA, an inhibitor that damps down activity in certain areas of the brain, as well as communication between them. These areas include the frontal and parietal regions, at the front and towards the back of the head.
Sanders’s colleagues recently used a form of non-invasive brain stimulation to demonstrate this principle in action, with propofol silencing the waves of activity you would normally see spreading across the brain in response to the stimulation.
“It’s very possible that anesthesia interferes with that ascending transmission of information,” he says. And without it, the mind temporarily disintegrates, becoming a blank screen with no ability to process or respond to the body’s signals.
In the clinic, there are many complicating factors to consider, of course. An anesthetist may choose to use one drug to induce the temporary coma and another to maintain it, and they need to consider many factors – such as the patient’s age and weight, whether they smoke or take drugs, the nature of their illness – to determine the doses.
Some people have a naturally higher threshold for anesthesia
Many procedures also use muscle relaxants. For example, nearly half of general anesthetics administered in the UK included intramuscular blockers. These drugs temporarily paralyze the body, preventing spasms and reflexes that could interfere with the surgery, without raising the dose of the anesthetic drugs to dangerously high levels.
Neuromuscular blockers can also ease the insertion of a tube through the windpipe, which can be used to ensure the airway remains open as well as to deliver oxygen and drugs, and to prevent stomach acid from entering the lungs. If the paralytic agents also stop muscles in the diaphragm and abdomen from moving, however, the patient’s breathing must be assisted artificially with a ventilator.
This all makes anesthesia as much art as science, and in the vast majority of cases, it works astonishingly well. More than 170 years after Morton’s public demonstration, anesthetists across the world plunge millions of people each year into comas and then bring them out safely. This doesn’t just reduce patients’ immediate suffering; many of the most invasive lifesaving procedures would simply not be possible without good general anesthesia.
But as with any medical procedure, there can be complicating factors. Some people may have a naturally higher threshold for anesthesia, meaning that the drugs don’t reduce the brain’s activity enough to dim the light of consciousness.
In some cases, such as injuries involving heavy bleeding, an anesthetist may be forced to use a lower dose of the anesthetic for the patient’s own safety.
It may also be difficult to time the effects of the different drugs, to ensure that the so-called induction dose – which gets you to sleep – doesn’t fade before the maintenance dose – to keep you unconscious – kicks in.
In some situations, you might be able to raise or lower your limb, or even speak, to show the anesthetic is not working before the surgeon picks up their scalpel. But if you have also been given neuromuscular blockers, that won’t be possible. The unfortunate result is that a small proportion of people may lie awake for part or all of their surgery without any ability to signal their distress.
Donna tells me about her own experience, during a lengthy telephone conversation from her home in Canada.
She says that she had felt anxious in the run-up to the operation, but she had had general anesthetic before without any serious problems. She was wheeled into the operating theater, placed on the operating table, and received the first dose of anesthesia. She soon drifted off to sleep, thinking, “Here I go.”
When she woke up, she could hear the nurses buzzing around the table, and she felt someone scrubbing at her abdomen – but she assumed that the operation was over and they were just clearing up. “I was thinking, ‘Oh boy, you were anxious for no reason.’” It was only once she heard the surgeon asking the nurse for a scalpel that the truth suddenly dawned on her: the operation wasn’t over. It hadn’t even begun.
The next thing she knew, she felt the blade of his knife against her belly as he made his first incision, leading to excruciating pain. She tried to sit up and to speak – but thanks to a neuromuscular blocker, her body was paralyzed. “I felt so… so powerless. There was just nothing I could do. I couldn’t move, couldn’t scream, couldn’t open my eyes,” she says. “I tried to cry just to get tears rolling down my cheeks, thinking that they would notice that and notice that something was going on. But I couldn’t make tears.”
The frustration was immense. “It felt like someone was sitting on me and holding me down and there was absolutely nothing I could do.”
Eventually, she tried to focus all her attention on moving one foot, which she managed to wiggle very slightly – and felt astonishing relief when one of the nurses placed his hand over it. Before she could move it again, however, the nurse had let go. She tried a total of three times, all with the same result. “It was very frustrating for me knowing that was the only way to communicate and it wasn’t working.”
I felt so… so powerless. There was just nothing I could do. I couldn’t move, couldn’t scream, couldn’t open my eyes.
Donna’s torment should have finished after the surgeon had ended his work. But as the neuromuscular blockers began to wear off, she started to move her tongue around the tube stuck down her throat; it was a way, she thought, of signalling to the staff that she was awake.
Unfortunately, the staff misread her attempts at communication, and began to withdraw the tube prematurely, before the paralytic agent had faded enough for her lungs to be able to operate on their own. “So here I was lying on the table and he took away my life support, my oxygen, I could not take a breath,” Donna says. She assumed she would die.
At this point, the operating room began to feel more distant, as she felt her mind escape in an out-of-body experience. A committed Christian, she says she felt the presence of God with her. It was only after the staff restored her oxygen supply that she drifted back into the operating room, to wake, crying.
That pain, the fear, the sense of absolute helplessness all still linger to this day – feelings of trauma that have led her to be put on medical leave from her job. And that has meant a loss of independence and confidence, and the abandonment of many of the hopes and dreams that she had built with her husband. “It’s hard to sit at home here and watch all the neighbors hurrying out of their house in the morning, jump in their cars, and go off to work, and I can’t.”
Various projects around the world have attempted to document experiences like Donna’s, but the Anesthesia Awareness Registry at the University of Washington, Seattle, offers some of the most detailed analyses. Founded in 2007, it has now collected more than 340 reports – most from North America – and although these reports are confidential, some details have been published, and they make illuminating reading.
Nearly all the patients included said they heard voices or other sounds under general anesthesia (patients’ eyes are typically closed during surgery so visual experiences tend to be less common).
“I heard the type of music and tried to figure why my surgeon chose that,” one patient told the registry. “I heard several voices around me. They seemed to be panicking. I heard them say they were losing me,” another reported.
As you might expect, a large majority of the accounts – more than 70 per cent – also contain reports of pain. “I felt the sting and burning sensation of four incisions being made, like a sharp knife cutting a finger,” wrote one. “Then searing, unbearable pain.”
“There were two parts I remember quite clearly,” wrote a patient who had had a wide hole made in his femur. “I heard the drill, felt the pain, and felt the vibration all the way up to my hip. The next part was the movement of my leg and the pounding of the ‘nail’.” The pain, he said, was “unlike anything I thought possible”.
It is the paralyzing effects of the muscle blockers that many find most distressing, however. For one thing, it produces the sensations that you are not breathing – which one patient described as “too horrible to endure”.
Then there’s the helplessness. Another patient noted: “I was screaming in my head things like ‘don’t they know I’m awake, open your eyes to signal them’.”
To make matters worse, all of this panic can be compounded by a lack of understanding of why they are awake but unable to move. “They have no reference point to say why is this happening,” says Christopher Kent at the University of Washington, who co-authored the paper about these accounts. The result, he says, is that many patients come to fear that they are dying. “Those are the worst of the anesthesia experiences.”
Estimates of how often anesthesia awareness happens have varied depending on the methods used, but those relying on patient reports had tended to suggest it was very rare indeed.
One of the largest and most thorough investigations was the fifth National Audit Project carried out by British and Irish anesthetists’ associations, in which every public hospital in the UK and Ireland had to report any incidents of awareness for a year. The results, published in 2014, found that the overall prevalence was just 1 in 19,000 patients undergoing anesthesia. The figure was higher – around 1 in 8,000 – if the anesthesia included paralyzing drugs, which is to be expected, since they prevent the patient from alerting the anesthetist that there is a problem before it is too late.
Many more people might be conscious during surgery, but they simply can’t remember it afterwards
These low numbers were comforting news. As the media reported at the time, you were more likely to die during surgery than to awake during it, confirming many doctors’ suspicions that this was a very remote risk.
Unfortunately, these figures are probably underestimates, as Peter Odor explains to me at St George’s Hospital in London. For one thing, the National Audit Project relied on patients themselves reporting directly to the hospital – but many people may feel unable or unwilling to come forward, and would instead prefer to just put the experience behind them.
There are also the amnesiac effects of the drugs themselves. “Unaesthetic drugs disrupt your ability to encode a memory,” said Odor. “And the dose that you give to obliterate memories is lower than that you need to obliterate consciousness. So memory goes well before consciousness goes.”
The result is that many more people might be conscious during surgery, but they simply can’t remember it afterwards.
To investigate this phenomenon, researchers are using what they call the isolated forearm technique. During the induction of the anesthesia, the staff place a cuff around the patient’s upper arm that delays the passage of the neuromuscular agent through the arm. This means that, for a brief period, the patient is still able to move their hand. So a member of staff could ask them to squeeze their hand in response to two questions: whether they were still aware, and, if so, whether they felt any pain. (Read more in this short on how doctors are trying to detect anaesthesia awareness.)
In the largest study of this kind to date, Robert Sanders at the University of Wisconsin–Madison recently collaborated with colleagues at six hospitals in the US, Europe and New Zealand. Of the 260 patients studied, 4.6 per cent responded to the experimenters’ first question, about awareness.
That is hundreds of times greater than the rate of remembered awareness events that had been noted in the National Audit Project. And around four in ten of those patients who did respond with the hand squeeze – 1.9 per cent across the whole group – also reported feeling pain in the experimenters’ second question.
These results raise some ethical quandaries. “Whenever I talk to the trainees I talk about the philosophical element to this,” says Sanders. “If the patient doesn’t remember, is it concerning?”
Sanders says that there’s no evidence that the patients who respond during the isolated forearm experiments, but fail to remember the experience later, do go on to develop PTSD or other psychological issues like Donna. And without those long-term consequences, you might conclude that the momentary awareness is unfortunate, but unalarming.
Yet the study does make him uneasy, and so he conducted a survey to gather the public’s views on the matter. Opinions were mixed. “Most people didn’t think that amnesia alone is sufficient – but a surprisingly large minority thought that as long as you didn’t remember the event, it’s OK,” Sanders says.
“My view is that the patient is expecting to be unconscious, and, as a researcher who wants to understand the mechanisms at play, but also a clinician who wants to deliver high-quality care and meet the expectations of the patient, we are duty-bound to understand this balance and to find out the true rates and the true impact of those events, whether they have any impact or not, and the ways we can curtail them.”
Given that the vast majority of patients will emerge from general anesthesia without traumatic memories, there is the danger that reports of anesthesia awareness – including this one – will needlessly increase anxiety before operations.
In the worst-case scenario, those fears could even prevent someone from having an essential medical procedure. Certainly anesthetists such as Sanders have emphasized that the risks of explicit recall are small, but if you are anxious you should talk to the hospital staff about your concerns.
But there are, nevertheless, strong arguments for making this phenomenon more widely known. For instance, as the reports from the University of Washington’s registry show, some patients’ distress was amplified by their lack of understanding of what was happening. They assumed that their awareness was a sign that they were dying. Perhaps if they had known the risk beforehand, that panic might have been assuaged.
A better understanding of anesthesia awareness might also help medical staff to deal with patients who have experienced this trauma. Many – including Donna – have felt that their accounts were misunderstood or simply dismissed by medical professionals.
The Washington registry, for instance, found that 75 percent of those who had reported awareness were unsatisfied with the response, with 51 percent saying that neither the anesthetist nor the surgeon expressed sympathy for their experience. Overall, just 10 percent received an apology, and only 15 percent were referred for counselling to help them to deal with the trauma.
There is the danger that reports of anaesthesia awareness – including this one – will needlessly increase anxiety before operations.
Donna says that many of the staff at her hospital seemed completely bewildered by her trauma. As she came around, she tried to explain to the nurses what had happened to her, but they just stood in silence, she says. “I’ll never forget their expressions – it was like they were in shock.” She puts this down to a lack of education and understanding of the phenomenon. “They don’t know how to handle this kind of a situation.”
Having gained strength in the years following the trauma, Donna is now trying to remedy the problem, by working with Canadian universities to educate doctors about the risks of anaesthesia awareness and the best ways to treat patients. “I want them to be prepared, because when things go wrong you need to know how you are going to react to the patient, because that is crucial to the patient’s recovery process.”
The ultimate goal, though, is to prevent these traumatic experiences from occurring in the first place, with studies using the isolated forearm technique helping to identify the best procedures to ensure unconsciousness. “There could be specific combinations of drugs that could produce the right blend of anaesthesia to insulate people from the external sensory world a bit better,” Sanders says.
There is even a chance that, as our understanding of the anaesthetised state deepens, we might be able to turn rudimentary unconscious responsiveness during surgery to our advantage. Certain forms of medical hypnosis have been found to have a real effect on patients’ experiences in controlled clinical trials – and the anaesthetic state could be the perfect time to put it into practice.
Although widespread signalling across the brain appears to be impaired when people are under general anaesthesia, there is evidence that certain areas – including the auditory cortex – remain responsive, suggesting that medical staff might be able to send suggestions and encouragement, while a patient is unconscious, to reduce their pain after surgery.
Studies investigating this possibility are few, but Jenny Rosendahl, at Jena University Hospital in Germany, and colleagues have attempted to gather all the evidence to date. Their meta-analysis showed a small but significant improvement in the patients’ ratings of post-operative nausea and vomiting, and less use of morphine after the operation.
Clearly, no one is suggesting that you would keep a patient fully aware on purpose, but perhaps one day more anaesthetists will be able to make use of the brain’s ability to absorb information on the operating table. It is an exciting thought that the words we hear during this mysterious twilight zone could have a lasting effect on our recovery.
Some evidence supports the use of microneedling for hair growth therapy. One study found microneedling…
A growing body of independent scientific research has identified significant risks associated with gas stoves,…
A couple of minutes of breath control can improve your physical and emotional health. It…
An 85-mile stretch that’s thronged with oil refineries and chemical plants has earned the nickname…
1958 Ferrari 250 GT Cabriolet Series I by Pinin Farina. Photo Courtesy of RM Sotheby's…
The Charles Kuonen Suspension Bridge, located in Switzerland, held the title of the world's longest…